- HTML Translated Version -
Metacognition:
Hypothetical Reasoning and Problem Solving
Authors:
Jaime Iván Ullauri Ullauri
Universidad Nacional de Educación, UNAE
Azogues, Ecuador
Carol Ivone Ullauri Ullauri
Universidad Nacional de Educación, UNAE
Azogues, Ecuador
Abstract
The
present work contemplates a precise and succinct theoretical reflection on the
metacognitive process of hypothetical reasoning as a proper capacity of the cognitive
development of children during the third childhood and how it affects the
logical-mathematical problem solving, on the cognitive perspective of Flavell
and Sternberg in communion with the resolving process of Polya's problems. This
investigation comprised the pertinent bibliographical study, having like
horizon to put in evidence the hypothetical reasoning processes in the
resolution of mathematical problems, concluding with the importance of the
teaching and development of basic cognitive processes in education that foment
the sprouting of metacognitive processes, allowing the child will work on his
task, rethink it and, as far as possible, solve it. From this perspective, this
work constitutes an overview of easy access and understanding for new professionals
who specialize in psychology and education.
Keywords: cognition; thinking; reasoning; problem solving.
Date Received: 02-11-2017 |
Date Acceptance: 08-01-2018 |
Metacognición: Razonamiento
Hipotético y Resolución de Problemas
Resumen
El presente trabajo contempla una reflexión teórica precisa
y sucinta sobre el proceso metacognitivo de razonamiento hipotético como
capacidad propia del desarrollo cognitivo de los niños durante la tercera
infancia y como este incide en la resolución de problemas lógico-matemáticos,
sobre la perspectiva cognitiva de Flavell y Sternberg en comunión con el
proceso resolutorio de problemas de Polya. Esta
investigación comprendió el estudio bibliográfico pertinente, teniendo como
horizonte poner en evidencia los procesos de razonamiento hipotético en la
resolución de problemas matemáticos, concluyendo con la importancia de la
enseñanza y desarrollo de procesos cognitivos básicos en educación que fomenten
el surgimiento de procesos metacognitivos, permitiendo al niño trabajar sobre
su tarea, repensarla y en lo posible solucionarla. Desde esta perspectiva este
trabajo constituye una panorámica de fácil acceso y comprensión para los
profesionales noveles que se especializan en psicología y educación.
Palabras clave: cognición; pensamiento; razonamiento; resolución de
problemas.
Fecha de Recepción: 02-11-2017 |
Fecha de Aceptación: 08-01-2018 |
1.
Introduction
The present
work leads to a theoretical approach to the cognitive and metacognitive
development of children during the third childhood (7-11 years of age) in
correspondence with the theoretical foundations established by Piaget (1991a)
and Flavell (2000a). The ultimate goal of this work is to highlight the most
relevant theoretical aspects of the development of logical reasoning as a
metacognitive process in solving mathematical problems in children.
From the
cognitive theory of Flavell (2000b): conceptual categories and evolutionary
stages of thought are established, that cognitive transition comprised between
the second and third childhood, so from this perspective the author considers
that metacognitive knowledge can lead the child to assimilate stimuli that are
in their context, assimilation that necessarily matches their interests,
capabilities and goals. Sternberg (2011a): states that these abilities have to
make clear the compression as the genesis of the process that the child
develops to solve a problem, this problematic compression is that through the
evocation-memory leads to the activation of prior knowledge, from which
different solutions can be produced to the same problem.
2.
Theory and concepts
2.1.
Cognition
It is
important to clarify basic concepts that allow building a panoramic view of
cognitive development and its implications for the child's integral
development. Flavell (2000c): integrates within cognitive development the
development of higher mental processes "corresponding to psychological
entities such as knowledge or knowledge (knowledge), consciousness,
intelligence, thinking, imagine, create, generate plans and strategies, reason,
infer, solve problems, conceptualize, classify and relate, symbolize and
possibly fantasize and dream" (Flavell, 2000d, p.13). From another
perspective Dorsch (2005): covers the conception of cognition and its
development as a generic term to name all the "processes or structures
that are related to consciousness and knowledge, such as perception, memory
(recognition), representation, the concept, the thought, and also the
conjecture, the expectation, the plan" (p.121).
In
conjunction with these definitions, Ullauri conceptualizes cognition as:
Cognition is not just a process or a set of processes
that enable the human being to solve their problems, cognition is a set of
skills and competencies that allow the human being to be 'capable of', this
ability translates into being able to outline and establish logical
relationships, which can only be made by the human being as such (Ullauri,
2013a, p.18).
In this way
"capable of" it is determined that the child can logically establish
strategies for solving problems "cognition must be understood not only as
a mental unit of intelligence but as a general unit of thought, which allows to
the human being to build and rebuild" (Ullauri, 2013b, p.18).
2.2.
Metacognition
For Bruner
(1995): metacognition is first of all a skill, which allows the child to think
about his or her thinking, making it possible to be aware of the situation in order
to solve it, also indicating the classification by levels of thought: in the
first level there are basic thought processes that are innate, in the second
level we find the ability to remember and adequate strategies to be literate
culturally and in the third level the thought processes and strategies that are
consciously evoked.
From the
perspective of Flavell (2000e): considers metacognition as the fourth level,
the "highest level" of the mental activity of the child, which
involves the most complex processes of thought, typical of the conscious mind.
For the author in the third childhood the basic aspects in which he frames the
metacognition are: a). own cognitive abilities, b). the tasks, c). the
metacognitive strategies. Also for Hacker (1998a): metacognition integrates:
a). knowledge and b). Metacognitive regulation, understood as the first
knowledge that is possessed on knowledge itself, which also involves their
cognitive and affective states, interpreting and reinterpreting them, while
metacognitive regulation is the process by which the child is able to control
and regulate cognitive processes.
While from
the Wellman theory of mind (1985), postulates stages in which knowledge and
metacognitive regulation are constructed are: i). existence, ii). process
distinction, iii). integration, iv). knowledge of the variables and v).
cognitive monitoring.
3. Cognitive development in the second and third
childhood
The main
characteristic of the child's thinking in this stage of development is the
advancement to a logical thought, in which he abandons intuition and
self-centeredness. Thus from this perspective Piaget, includes at this stage
the concrete operational stage, a space of development in which children manage
to master the notions of conservation, transitivity, class inclusion, multiple
classification and seriation, as well as initiating the understanding of the
reversibility, identity and notions and understanding of logical concepts
(Piaget, 1991b, p.54).
Flavell
(2000f): in the stage of concrete formal operations, he visualizes the
cognitive processes that occur during the second and third childhood in the
following way: in the second childhood cognitive processes are presented as i).
Perceived appearances, ii). centrations, iii). states and iv). irreversibility,
while in correspondence to each of them in the third childhood they are already
developed as: i). inferred reality, ii). decentration, iii). transformations,
iv). reversibility, determining that the nature of the hypothetical reasoning process
is metacognitive and that it occurs from the third childhood.
During the
second childhood (2-7 years), a development in the basic competences of a
progressive way with presence of the intuition and egocentrism is evidenced, in
the third childhood (7-11 years), the concrete operations and the logical
thought are potentialized , as Flavell (2000g) states: "the development of
these that advances from the total absence of competencies to the presence of
the most advanced" (p.74). Around this aspect Flavell also indicates that
the centering, the perceived appearances and the irreversibility are conditions
of the second childhood, whereas the decentration, the inferred reality and
reversibility are conditions of the third childhood.
3.1.
Metacognition
The development
of metacognitive processes leads the individual to work on meta-knowledge,
which Flavell (2000h) defines as "the ability to control and evaluate
one's current memory capabilities" (p.145); and as the capacity of the
"cooking on knowledge". So it is in the third childhood the space in
which metacognitive activities are developed.
For Flavell
(2000i): it is necessary to understand that meta-knowledge involves the
knowledge and metacognitive experiences that the child internalizes about the
cognitive contexts in which it develops, such knowledge has more the
characteristic of being declarative than procedural, subdividing itself into
knowledge about the people, tasks and strategies:
The
knowledge about people involves any type of knowledge that the child can
internalize and these differ between one person and another, the knowledge of
the task includes the information that is available in the context in which the
child is to be assimilated, this information is generated from the
metacognitive experience, while the knowledge about the strategies involves the
ability of the child in our case to identify the cognitive processes involved
in the problem-solving process (Flavell, 2000j, p.147).
Thus, it is
understood that a cognitive strategy contributes to the realization of the
cognitive activity in question, while the metacognitive strategy is the one
that will generate the information about the work that is being developed or in
itself the progress of the own strategy.
3.2.
Metacognitive experience
The metacognitive
experience translates as "the bulk of metacognitive knowledge, really
refers to combinations or interactions between two or three of the
categories" (Flavell, 2000k, p.148), and by its nature this can be short
or long, as also of a simple or complex nature depending on the content that
these entail, these cognitive experiences can occur throughout the cognitive
process that is undertaken for the construction of a task or the resolution of
a problem.
3.3.
Problem
resolution
Solving
problems in the daily life of the human being is necessarily daily, in any
context that is found, so it is necessary that the child can establish a
resolution plan, in which not necessarily the main objective is the effective
solution or not of the problem, but the process that the child develops within
that plan that he established with the intention of solving it, in other words,
to be aware of the different steps he takes to solve the problem, the
conscience that the child has of this process allows you to go back over each
of the phases of the work you have developed with the aim of correcting it so
you can consider that:
any obstacle situation that facilitates the activation
of a cognitive process, which to be solved requires the interaction of actions
that can be executed, based on skills such as: observation, inference,
assumption, analysis, etc., that the human being undertakes as such, allowing
the acquisition of new skills that enable the development of a process that
creates the way for children to establish logical and real solutions to the
problem (Ullauri, 2013c, p.46).
Next, we
address two problem solving models, the first proposed by Polya (1984a), and
the second one defined by Sternberg (2011b). The Polya model establishes four
phases for solving mathematical problems that are: i). understand the problem,
ii). set up a plan, iii). execute the plan, iv). look back (Polya, 1984b,
pp.51-53):
I.
Understanding the problem: this understanding of the
problem does not only imply its literal reading, its words, its signs and
symbols, but also the implicit, which at first glance is not seen, such as the
relationships, categories and variables that compose it, helping the child to
understand the problem in its entirety. (Polya,
1984c; Ullauri, 2013d).
The genesis of
the process implies reasoning about the task that is presented to solve it,
recognizing and understanding the problem in question, this implies that the
child must start from the critical analysis of relationships, categories and
variables to determine them in a first phase, it will not be possible that the
child reaches this stage if he has not been able to establish in sufficient
depth the characteristics of the problem.
II.
A second moment is the configuration of the plan,
which includes the mapping of the problem situation that needs to be solved,
the structuring of this plan requires the staging of the skills and competences
pertinent to the type of problem that is trying to be solved, in this case the
metacognitive process of hypothetical reasoning as a means that allows to look
in the future towards the construction of a visualization about the resolution
of the problem:
that as a video player can be reviewed forward and
backward, so that you can perceive the smallest details, which will allow you
to decide which strategy is the most appropriate for resolving the problem,
going back and reviewing again the scene and hypotheses (Ullauri, 2013e, p.57).
So it is
understood that the nature of hypothetical reasoning is metacognitive.
III.
After the configuration of the plan the execution
becomes. The execution of a plan does not guarantee the resolution of the
problem, which can not be classified as an error or failure but rather as an
opportunity to rethink the strategy, assess errors and start planning the new
plan and its execution.
The dynamics
of these phases as containing cognitive processes necessarily involves the
awareness of these by the child, which enables him to correct errors about the
situation. During the execution stage of the plan, it will be able to stop on
the fly and rethink the strategy that is not working correctly to continue.
IV.
In the end, looking back makes it possible to
discriminate whether the solution achieved is the correct solution or not, this
exercise that the child makes generates the space to be able to understand and
understand the problem, achieving the verification that satisfies him.
Polya (1984d): estimates that the process of solving
problems includes two types of reasoning; regressive reasoning and projective
reasoning, these two types of reasoning should be reflected in the
understanding of the problem, the constitution of the plan to solve it and
especially when the resolution of the problem is not generated to reformulate
the strategies to try to solve it (p.134).
From this perspective, the author considers that
Polya:
we look for from what background the desired result could be deduced;
then we look for what could be the antecedent of this antecedent, and so on, until,
passing from one antecedent to another, we finally find something known or
admitted as true. We call this process analysis, backward solution or
regressive reasoning. In the synthesis, on the contrary, reversing the process,
we start from the last point reached in the analysis, of the element already
known or admitted as true. We deduce what in the analysis preceded it and we
continue like this until, retracing our steps, we finally reach what we were
asked. We call this process synthesis, constructive solution or progressive
reasoning (Polya, 1984e, p.134).
The problem solving process proposed by Sternberg
(2011c): it is not necessarily oriented towards solving mathematical problems. This
process has a cyclical nature that starts from: 1). identification of the
problem, 2). definition and representation thereof, 3). formulation of
strategies, 4). organization of information, 5). location of resources, 6).
monitoring, 7). evaluation (p.430).
Figure 1. Sternberg
problem resolution cycle.
SEE IN THE
ORIGINAL VERSION
Source: Sternberg (2011, page 430).
The process of problem solving of Sternberg (2011d):
after the second phase does not maintain a sequential order, this particular
does not indicate that the ordering of these phases negatively or positively
affects the resolution of the problem, that is why from the The third phase up
to the sixth phase allows the creation of new metacognitive processes that
facilitate the restructuring of strategies to solve problems, that is, the
reversibility of thought.
In correspondence with the cognitive processes
determined by Flavell (2000l), inferred reality, decentration, transformations,
reversibility and those indicated by Polya (1984f): the regressive and
projective reasoning are corresponding, identifying these as necessary to be
able to solve a problem with complete satisfaction the child must have
developed these basic processes of metacognitive thinking to understand the
problem, configure the plan, establish resolution strategies, develop the plan
to solve it and in case of not being able to solve it, the regressive reasoning
will enable you to return to the plan and strategies , to establish
improvements or configure a new plan that allows you to solve the problem, in
this sense in a mathematical problem are involved all the cognitive processes
that we have reviewed, from this point the mathematical logical thinking plays
a decisive role.
3.4.
Mathematical
logical thinking
For Castaño (2010): "the development of
logical-mathematical thinking is the development of the ability to establish
relationships and to operate with them" (p.96), understanding that the
development of this type of thinking involves the involvement of cognitive
processes that allow the establishment of relationships. In this sense Piaget
(1991c): argues that "the child's thinking does not become logical only
through the organization of systems of operations that obey laws of common sets
that are; composition, reversibility, direct operation and its inverse,
association of operations" (p.71), from this perspective we can
understand" mathematical logical thinking as the cognitive and
metacognitive process generated by the interactions of experiences and actions
in the solution of problem" (Ullauri, 2013f, p.46).
According to Saguillo (2008a): the mathematical
logical thought is based on the nature and objective reality that is expressed
in propositions that acquire a mathematical value that can be false or true,
thus "the classical mathematical logical thought is articulated also
presupposing certain epistemic capacities of the Humans". (Saguillo,
2008b, p.6), which are necessarily binding with cognitive and metacognitive
processes, allowing different relations to be established in relation to
situations, objects and concepts, allowing the child to structure and
restructure reality, using cognitive processes and metacognitives such as the
approach of projective solutions to a specific task, in other words, the
hypothesis approach.
The resolution of a problem of any kind requires the
establishment of resolution strategies that are configured in a plan, which is
necessarily built on the basis of cognitive and metacognitive processes.
4.
Methodology
The present work
contemplated the pertinent bibliographical study on the development of logical
reasoning in the third childhood and how this metacognitive process is immersed
in the resolution of problems. The bibliographic inquiry was conducted between
the months of September and October 2017, for which some descriptors have been
used that include: cognitive development in the second and third childhood,
metacognition and metacognitive processes, hypothetical reasoning, mathematical
logical thinking and problem solving. From the sources consulted, those that
report on the basic aspects that show the cognitive and metacognitive
development of the children were selected and in a spatial way those that
implicitly are related to the resolution of mathematical problems..
5.
Conclusions
The
realization of this work has allowed to reach some key conclusions in the
process of development of hypothetical reasoning as a metacognitive process for
the resolution of problems, these conclusions are concretized in:
·
Metacognitive processes are the product of the sum of
cognitive processes, thus the emergence of hypothetical reasoning is inherently
linked to the sum of basic cognitive skills such as perception, attention,
memory, thinking, reasoning and language.
·
It is evident that the treatment of the tasks faced by
the children are considered by them as problems, which are not necessarily
known by them and that will most likely be solved by the development of some
cognitive skills such as: problem identification, capacity for definition and
representation, formulation of strategies, organization of information,
location of resources, monitoring the process, establishment of inferences,
evaluation and in a special way for the development of metacognitive dexterity
of hypothetical reasoning as a mathematical logical thinking process.
·
The theoretical studies of Piaget (1991d), Flavell
(2000m), Sternberg (2011e) and Hacker (1988b) are contrasted in relation to
Polya's (1984g) problem-solving perspective: it can be stated that the function
that the cognitive and metacognitive processes such as hypothetical reasoning
allows you to configure a plan and its strategies in a projective way to solve
problems of different kinds.
6.
References
Bruner, J. (1995). Escuelas para pensar. Madrid: Paidós.
Castaño, J. (2010). La matemática en Transición y Primer Grado de Escuela Nueva. Manual
de implementación escuela nueva, generalidades y Orientaciones Pedagógicas para
Transición y Primer Grado. Tomo I. Bogotá, Colombia: Ministerio de Educación
Nacional. ISBN: 978-958-8712-41-3, págs. 212. Recuperado de: https://www.mineducacion.gov.co/1759/articles-340089_archivopdf_orientaciones_pedagogicas_tomoI.pdf
Dorsch, F. (2005). Diccionario de Psicología. Barcelona: Heder.
Flavell, J. (2000a,b,c,d,e,f,g,h,i,j,k,l,m).
El Desarrollo cognitivo. Madrid:
Visor.
Hacker,
D. (1998a,b). Metacognition: Definitions and empirical foundations. En
Metacognition in educational theory and practice. EE. UU.: The University of Memphis, pp. 1-23. Recuperado
de: http://vcell.ndsu.nodak.edu/~ganesh/seminar/Hacker_Metacognition%20-%20Definitions%20and%20Empirical%20Foundations.htm
Piaget,
J. (1991a,b,c,d). Seis
estudios de Piscología. Barcelona: Labor.
Polya, G. (1984a,b,c,d,e,f,g). Cómo plantear y resolver problemas.
México: Trillas.
Saguillo, J. (2008a,b). El pensamiento lógico-matemático.
Madrid: Akal.
Sternberg, R. (2011a,b,c,d,e). Psicología cognitiva. México: Thomson.
Ullauri, J. (2013a,b,c,d,e,f). Proceso metacognitivo del pensamiento
lógico matemático: razonamiento hipotético. Cuenca, Ecuador: Universidad de
Cuenca, págs. 207.
Wellman, H. (1985). The origins of metacognition.
In D.L. Forrest-Pressley, G.E.
MacKinnon, & T.G. Waller (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition and human
performance, pp. 1-31. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Jaime Iván Ullauri
Ullauri
e-mail: jaime.ullauri@unae.edu.ec
Born in Cuenca, Ecuador. Lcdo. in Educational
Psychology, Master in Education and Development of Thought (Universidad de Cuenca-Ecuador),
Master in Educational Guidance (UNED-Spain) and PhD student of the Psychology
of Education program of the Universitat de Barcelona. Professor-Researcher in
the Chair of Human Learning and General Leveling at the Universidad Nacional de
Educación, has participated in similar processes at the Universidad de Cuenca.
Teacher of Basic General Education and Bachillerato in Social Studies and
Development of Philosophical Thought. Academic Facilitator within the programs
of Advisory and Educational Audit and Continuing Education "Yes
Profe" of the Ministry of Education of Ecuador.
Carol Ivone Ullauri
Ullauri
e-mail: carol.ullauri@unae.edu.ec
Born
in Cuenca, Ecuador. Bachelor in Educational Psychology Specialization in
Professional Guidance, Master in Educational Psychology and Human Development
in multicultural contexts (Universidad de Valencia-Spain). Professor-Researcher
in the Chair of Neurosciences at the Universidad Nacional de Educación.
Professor-Researcher in the Theories of Learning, Psychology of Human
Development, Theories and Psychological Systems and General Psychology at the
Universidad de Cuenca. Professor of Leveling knowledge at the Universidad de
Cuenca. Teacher of Basic General Education and Bachillerato in Language and
Literature.
The content of this manuscript is
disseminated under a Creative Commons License
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
- Original Version in Spanish -
DOI: https://doi.org/10.29394/Scientific.issn.2542-2987.2018.3.8.6.121-137